Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download

Posted on  by 



Outcome measures in mental health occupational therapy workshop 18th June 2019 Royal College of Occupational Therapy National Conference

  1. Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download Windows 7
  2. Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download
  3. Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download Windows 10
  4. Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download 64-bit

MOTION members Mandy Graham, Dr Mary Birken and Dr Catriona Connell hosted a conference workshop on outcome measures in mental health occupational therapy. A blog about the workshop can be found here: https://motionresearch.wordpress.com/2019/07/12/outcome-measures-in-mental-health-occupational-therapy-workshop/

Here’s the presentation and responses to questions we ask participants in groups and via Poll Everywhere.

  1. Research In Motion Limited is a designer, manufacturer, and marketer of wireless solutions for the worldwide mobile communications market.
  2. Research in Motion (RIM), founded in Waterloo, Ontario, first developed the Inter@ctive Pager 900, announced on September 18, 1996. The Inter@ctive Pager 900 was a clamshell-type device that allowed two-way paging.

In the early research of deep learning, it was observed that the initial layers of the networks learn the very fundamental features related to images such as edge representations and those kinds of features are used frequently in many computer vision tasks including motion estimation.

Poll Everywhere
At the start of the workshop we used Poll Everywhere, a mobile phone application, to ask three questions and gather their responses instantly.
Poll Question 1: What do you think of when we say outcome measures – 33 responses

Here’s what they said: -A large array of standardised, non standardised, hard stats -MOHOST
-Evidencing change, improvement – Inconsistent use
-Measurement of change / progress of intervention
-Baseline measures effectiveness of interventions -Monitoring performance
-Assessment used prior to activity and after an activity to gage success / person satisfaction / progress etc -Results of intervention
-How we measure how successful / effective something has been
-Measurement of change, standardised or non-standardised. Way of showing standard of service. -AusTOMS -Tools we can use to measure the effect our work has had
-The impact / success of intervention -Something to measure progress against
-Ensuring quality – Demonstrates effectiveness of intervention
-Not used enough -Measuring the efficacy of interventions
-Reporting on evidence -Measuring change / progress -Measuring the outcome of an applied -intervention -Demonstrating impact of an intervention, should be person centred -A way of evaluating an intervention -Measuring efficacy -Justify need for therapy -evidence base -How did we do? -Standardised vs non-standardised -MOHO
-Standardised -Measuring the success of interventions -Measures of effectiveness

Poll Question 2. Do you currently use outcome measures – 23 reponses
Always 17%
Sometimes 78%
Rarely 4%

Poll Question 3. What outcome measures do you use – 40 (the word cloud capped at 40) The larger the word, the more respondents used it.

We asked participants at the workshop the following questions. Participants were sitting in groups with colleagues with the same service user group and answered the questions together.
Below are their answers
Question 1 Who are you measuring outcomes for?

Acute Inpatient
-Future groups/service users -Unmet needs of service users
-Keep managers happy! -Helps me as an OT!- guides practice and intervention
-Evidence in other reports -Valuable to handover to next service/person/therapist/other OTs. -Helps me organise my thoughts as an OT in MDT.

Specialist (managers/students)
-Service user -Managers -Commissioners -Quality measure
-Self (self-efficacy) -MDT –reasoning -Research
-Public health -Funding
-Influence local and national developments-service improvements
-For carers -For local communities

Community
CCG-commissioning evidence
Service user- with involvement
Service effectiveness-staffing increase

Forensic
-Patients -Wider clinical team/MDT/OT service
-Care and Treatment Reviews -Ourselves- Commissioners

Question 2 What is the outcome you are trying to achieve?

Acute Inpatient
-Level of functioning -Discharge needs
Any other service referrals needed? -Justification for the service
-Unmet needs? -Interests and starting points?
-Level of improvement -Identifying day to day changes
-Client satisfaction with services
Justifying need for further intervention
Specialist (managers/students)
-Does it work? (find out if it works?) -Effectiveness
-Occupational change of all types -Occupational satisfaction
– Experience of client/carer -Quality of life
-Engagement -Participation -Value for money
-Impact of intervention (what difference does it make?)
-Targets that have been set

Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download

Forensic
-Reduction in risk -Change over time -Quality and efficacy of intervention?
-Perceived readiness -Overall occupational performance
-Attaining treatment goals eg able to come out of room, increase in pro social behaviours/strategies.
Community
-Impact on wellbeing -? it depends! What are service users goals?
-Optimizing function and occupational participation
-Indication of a positive result -Quality of life -That the intervention is successful

Question 3What outcome measures are available?

Acute inpatient
-MOHOST -OSA -OCAIRS -ESI –Observation -KAWA -Sleep diaries -Mood scales -APOM -Allens cognitive scale -AMPS
-Goal attainment scaling -COTES -Interest checklist

Specialist (mixed group)
-MOHOST -OSA -AMPS -GAS/GAS LIGHT -COPM
-ACIS -OTHER MOHO -Allen Cognitive Levels Screen
-Functional Independence Measure (FIM) -Functional Assessment tool (FAM)
-Therapy Outcome Measures (TOMS) -RECOVERY STAR -Non standardised
-Receipts-Timed and date stamped photos -Likert scales -Carer report
-Users report -Case studies and stories/verbal narrative
-Narrative occupational assessment (repeated) -Binary individualised measures GA/GNA/GPA

Community
-ACIS -Interest checklist -MOHOST -HONOS -WEMWBS
-AMPS -OCAIRS -MOCA -MMSC
-Depression scales -Recovery star

Forensic
-AMPS -MOHOST -APOM -VDTMOCA- ACIS -COTES
-OSA -TOMS -OCAIRS -GAD-7
-LSP life skills profile -Self rating scales -COPM
-Goal attainment scaling -VQ

On Tuesday, 13 March 2018 a OTalk twitter discussion was hosted by Tori on behalf of MOTION:
Topic: What does the term participation mean in mental health from an Occupational Therapy perspective?

Transcript for the Otalk listed below can be found here:
#OTalk 13th March – What does the term participation mean in mental health?

To generate wider discussions regarding participation as defined by ICF as an outcome for mental health and occupational therapy in mental health, three network members presented posters at two conferences in 2017; Royal College of Occupational Therapy Specialist Section- Mental Health and ENMESH (European Network for Mental Health Service Evaluation.

CONNELL, C., BIRKEN, M. & INMAN, J. 2017. The World Health Organization’s concept of participation: A proposal for assessing mental health and evaluating outcomes in real world settings. Conceptualising, measuring and influencing context in mental health care: From the individual to society. ENMESH Groningen, The Netherlands.

RCOTSS-MH conference poster 2017:

CONNELL, C., BIRKEN, M. & INMAN, J. 2017. Advancing intervention effectiveness research in mental health occupational therapy. Royal College of Occupational Therapists Specialist Section – Mental Health Conference. Birmingham, UK.

WFOT congress 2018 Poster:

Download

Inman, J., Birken, M., Bannigan, K., Connell, C., Morris, K. & Smyth, G. 2018. Effectiveness of occupational therapy mental health interventions: Advancing research to develop the evidence base. World Federation of Occupational Therapy Congress, Cape Town, South Africa.

Survey of network members

In November 2017 MOTION carried out a survey of the then current membership with the aim of seeking MOTION members views on MOTION aims, and focus of the six current working groups, suggest new aims or working groups, and to identify their available time to commit to MOTION activities.

The survey was open between 27/11/17-19/02/18

16 members responded:

Demographics of respondents (n=16)
Lecturer 4
Course director 1
Post PhD Researcher 3
OT student 1
Mental Health Service User Researcher 1
PhD student 5
Clinician 5
AHP Darzi fellowship 1
Reader 1

Working groups signed up to
Participation as an outcome and measures of participation 13
Networking and dissemination 7
Economic evaluation within occupational therapy randomised controlled trials 6
Profile development & building website 1
Funding to support network activity 1
Important goals for MOTION identified by respondents that are not already stated

Developing a research programme
• Developing a research programme for the creation of occupation-focused and/or based interventions with participation outcomes.
• supporting the development of programmes of research (my sense is that this is an issue, beyond the bounds of MOTION, for occupational therapy in general)
• Small monies bid to Deliver collaborative project as basis for larger funding bids. Publish these projects.
Influence development of mental health occupational therapy research
• Influence research priorities at RCOT and other mental health research funders
• Influence other researchers to conduct applied research, on outcomes and interventions

Broader membership
• Have family member/carer members however they would like to define themselves as a representative.

Develop collaborations
• Establishing collaborative partnerships with Universities and health providers

Directly support the increase of research capacity in Mental health occupational therapy interventions research
• Collating resources to strengthen practice.
• to increase research capacity within the mental health OT workforce

Summary of what respondents bring to the working groups (e.g. skills) that they have signed up to?
Connections with OTs involved in sleep research and practice across the globe
I work daily with MH clients, so it is easy to gather data from (&with) them
Understanding of children and young people’s mental health
Concept analysis work on concept of participation
Research skills:
design studies
apply for funding
systematic review skills
intervention development
Past member of an NHS REC
Knowledge of clinical academic networks

What is research? Depending on who you ask, you will likely get very different answers to this seemingly innocuous question. Some people will say that they routinely research different online websites to find the best place to buy goods or services they want. Television news channels supposedly conduct research in the form of viewer polls on topics of public interest such as forthcoming elections or government-funded projects. Undergraduate students research the Internet to find the information they need to complete assigned projects or term papers. Graduate students working on research projects for a professor may see research as collecting or analyzing data related to their project. Businesses and consultants research different potential solutions to remedy organizational problems such as a supply chain bottleneck or to identify customer purchase patterns. However, none of the above can be considered “scientific research” unless: (1) it contributes to a body of science, and (2) it follows the scientific method. This chapter will examine what these terms mean.

Science

What is science? To some, science refers to difficult high school or college-level courses such as physics, chemistry, and biology meant only for the brightest students. To others, science is a craft practiced by scientists in white coats using specialized equipment in their laboratories. Etymologically, the word “science” is derived from the Latin word scientia meaning knowledge. Science refers to a systematic and organized body of knowledge in any area of inquiry that is acquired using “the scientific method” (the scientific method is described further below). Science can be grouped into two broad categories: natural science and social science. Natural science is the science of naturally occurring objects or phenomena, such as light, objects, matter, earth, celestial bodies, or the human body. Natural sciences can be further classified into physical sciences, earth sciences, life sciences, and others. Physical sciences consist of disciplines such as physics (the science of physical objects), chemistry (the science of matter), and astronomy (the science of celestial objects). Earth sciences consist of disciplines such as geology (the science of the earth). Life sciences include disciplines such as biology (the science of human bodies) and botany (the science of plants). In contrast, social science is the science of people or collections of people, such as groups, firms, societies, or economies, and their individual or collective behaviors. Social sciences can be classified into disciplines such as psychology (the science of human behaviors), sociology (the science of social groups), and economics (the science of firms, markets, and economies).

The natural sciences are different from the social sciences in several respects. The natural sciences are very precise, accurate, deterministic, and independent of the person m aking the scientific observations. For instance, a scientific experiment in physics, such as measuring the speed of sound through a certain media or the refractive index of water, should always yield the exact same results, irrespective of the time or place of the experiment, or the person conducting the experiment. If two students conducting the same physics experiment obtain two different values of these physical properties, then it generally means that one or both of those students must be in error. However, the same cannot be said for the social sciences, which tend to be less accurate, deterministic, or unambiguous. For instance, if you measure a person’s happiness using a hypothetical instrument, you may find that the same person is more happy or less happy (or sad) on different days and sometimes, at different times on the same day. One’s happiness may vary depending on the news that person received that day or on the events that transpired earlier during that day. Furthermore, there is not a single instrument or metric that can accurately measure a person’s happiness. Hence, one instrument may calibrate a person as being “more happy” while a second instrument may find that the same person is “less happy” at the same instant in time. In other words, there is a high degree of measurement error in the social sciences and there is considerable uncertainty and little agreement on social science policy decisions. For instance, you will not find many disagreements among natural scientists on the speed of light or the speed of the earth around the sun, but you will find numerous disagreements among social scientists on how to solve a social problem such as reduce global terrorism or rescue an economy from a recession. Any student studying the social sciences must be cognizant of and comfortable with handling higher levels of ambiguity, uncertainty, and error that come with such sciences, which merely reflects the high variability of social objects.

Sciences can also be classified based on their purpose. Basic sciences , also called pure sciences, are those that explain the most basic objects and forces, relationships between them, and laws governing them. Examples include physics, mathematics, and biology. Applied sciences , also called practical sciences, are sciences that apply scientific knowledge from basic sciences in a physical environment. For instance, engineering is an applied science that applies the laws of physics and chemistry for practical applications such as building stronger bridges or fuel efficient combustion engines, while medicine is an applied science that applies the laws of biology for solving human ailments. Both basic and applied sciences are required for human development. However, applied sciences cannot stand on their own right, but instead relies on basic sciences for its progress. Of course, the industry and private enterprises tend to focus more on applied sciences given their practical value, while universities study both basic and applied sciences.

Scientific Knowledge

The purpose of science is to create scientific knowledge. Scientific knowledge refers to a generalized body of laws and theories to explain a phenomenon or behavior of interest that are acquired using the scientific method. Laws are observed patterns of phenomena or behaviors, while theories are systematic explanations of the underlying phenomenon or behavior. For instance, in physics, the Newtonian Laws of Motion describe what happens when an object is in a state of rest or motion (Newton’s First Law), what force is needed to move a stationary object or stop a moving object (Newton’s Second Law), and what happens when two objects collide (Newton’s Third Law). Collectively, the three laws constitute the basis of classical mechanics – a theory of moving objects. Likewise, the theory of optics explains the properties of light and how it behaves in different media, electromagnetic theory explains the properties of electricity and how to generate it, quantum mechanics explains the properties of subatomic particles, and thermodynamics explains the properties of energy and mechanical work. An introductory college level text book in physics will likely contain separate chapters devoted to each of these theories. Similar theories are also available in social sciences. For instance, cognitive dissonance theory in psychology explains how people react when their observations of an event is different from what they expected of that event, general deterrence theory explains why some people engage in improper or criminal behaviors, such as illegally download music or commit software piracy, and the theory of planned behavior explains how people make conscious reasoned choices in their everyday lives.

The goal of scientific research is to discover laws and postulate theories that can explain natural or social phenomena, or in other words, build scientific knowledge. It is important to understand that this knowledge may be imperfect or even quite far from the truth. Sometimes, there may not be a single universal truth, but rather an equilibrium of “multiple truths.” We must understand that the theories, upon which scientific knowledge is based, are only explanations of a particular phenomenon, as suggested by a scientist. As such, there may be good or poor explanations, depending on the extent to which those explanations fit well with reality, and consequently, there may be good or poor theories. The progress of science is marked by our progression over time from poorer theories to better theories, through better observations using more accurate instruments and more informed logical reasoning.

We arrive at scientific laws or theories through a process of logic and evidence. Logic (theory) and evidence (observations) are the two, and only two, pillars upon which scientific knowledge is based. In science, theories and observations are interrelated and cannot exist without each other. Theories provide meaning and significance to what we observe, and observations help validate or refine existing theory or construct new theory. Any other means of knowledge acquisition, such as faith or authority cannot be considered science.

Scientific Research

Research in motion network & wireless cards driver downloads

Given that theories and observations are the two pillars of science, scientific research operates at two levels: a theoretical level and an empirical level. The theoretical level is concerned with developing abstract concepts about a natural or social phenomenon and relationships between those concepts (i.e., build “theories”), while the empirical level is concerned with testing the theoretical concepts and relationships to see how well they reflect our observations of reality, with the goal of ultimately building better theories. Over time, a theory becomes more and more refined (i.e., fits the observed reality better), and the science gains maturity. Scientific research involves continually moving back and forth between theory and observations. Both theory and observations are essential components of scientific research. For instance, relying solely on observations for making inferences and ignoring theory is not considered valid scientific research.

Depending on a researcher’s training and interest, scientific inquiry may take one of two possible forms: inductive or deductive. In inductive research , the goal of a researcher is to infer theoretical concepts and patterns from observed data. In deductive research , the goal of the researcher is to test concepts and patterns known from theory using new empirical data. Hence, inductive research is also called theory-building research, and deductive research is theory-testing research. Note here that the goal of theory-testing is not just to test a theory, but possibly to refine, improve, and extend it. Figure 1.1 depicts the complementary nature of inductive and deductive research. Note that inductive and deductive research are two halves of the research cycle that constantly iterates between theory and observations. You cannot do inductive or deductive research if you are not familiar with both the theory and data components of research. Naturally, a complete researcher is one who can traverse the entire research cycle and can handle both inductive and deductive research.

It is important to understand that theory-building (inductive research) and theory-testing (deductive research) are both critical for the advancement of science. Elegant theories are not valuable if they do not match with reality. Likewise, mountains of data are also useless until they can contribute to the construction to meaningful theories. Rather than viewing these two processes in a circular relationship, as shown in Figure 1.1, perhaps they can be better viewed as a helix, with each iteration between theory and data contributing to better explanations of the phenomenon of interest and better theories. Though both inductive and deductive research are important for the advancement of science, it appears that inductive (theory-building) research is more valuable when there are few prior theories or explanations, while deductive (theory-testing) research is more productive when there are many competing theories of the same phenomenon and researchers are interested in knowing which theory works best and under what circumstances.

Theory building and theory testing are particularly difficult in the social sciences, given the imprecise nature of the theoretical concepts, inadequate tools to measure them, and the presence of many unaccounted factors that can also influence the phenomenon of interest. It is also very difficult to refute theories that do not work. For instance, Karl Marx’s theory of communism as an effective means of economic production withstood for decades, before it was finally discredited as being inferior to capitalism in promoting economic growth and social welfare. Erstwhile communist economies like the Soviet Union and China eventually moved toward more capitalistic economies characterized by profit-maximizing private enterprises. However, the recent collapse of the mortgage and financial industries in the United States demonstrates that capitalism also has its flaws and is not as effective in fostering economic growth and social welfare as previously presumed. Unlike theories in the natural sciences, social science theories are rarely perfect, which provides numerous opportunities for researchers to improve those theories or build their own alternative theories.

Conducting scientific research, therefore, requires two sets of skills – theoretical and methodological – needed to operate in the theoretical and empirical levels respectively. Methodological skills (“know-how”) are relatively standard, invariant across disciplines, and easily acquired through doctoral programs. However, theoretical skills (“know-what”) is considerably harder to master, requires years of observation and reflection, and are tacit skills that cannot be “taught” but rather learned though experience. All of the greatest scientists in the history of mankind, such as Galileo, Newton, Einstein, Neils Bohr, Adam Smith, Charles Darwin, and Herbert Simon, were master theoreticians, and they are remembered for the theories they postulated that transformed the course of science. Methodological skills are needed to be an ordinary researcher, but theoretical skills are needed to be an extraordinary researcher!

Scientific Method

In the preceding sections, we described science as knowledge acquired through a scientific method. So what exactly is the “scientific method”? Scientific method refers to a standardized set of techniques for building scientific knowledge, such as how to make valid observations, how to interpret results, and how to generalize those results. The scientific method allows researchers to independently and impartially test preexisting theories and prior findings, and subject them to open debate, modifications, or enhancements. The scientific method must satisfy four characteristics:

  • Replicability: Others should be able to independently replicate or repeat a scientific study and obtain similar, if not identical, results.
  • Precision: Theoretical concepts, which are often hard to measure, must be defined with such precision that others can use those definitions to measure those concepts and test that theory.
Cards
  • Falsifiability: A theory must be stated in a way that it can be disproven. Theories that cannot be tested or falsified are not scientific theories and any such knowledge is not scientific knowledge. A theory that is specified in imprecise terms or whose concepts are not accurately measurable cannot be tested, and is therefore not scientific. Sigmund Freud’s ideas on psychoanalysis fall into this category and is therefore not considered a

“theory”, even though psychoanalysis may have practical utility in treating certain types of ailments.

  • Parsimony: When there are multiple explanations of a phenomenon, scientists must always accept the simplest or logically most economical explanation. This concept is called parsimony or “Occam’s razor.” Parsimony prevents scientists from pursuing overly complex or outlandish theories with endless number of concepts and relationships that may explain a little bit of everything but nothing in particular.

Any branch of inquiry that does not allow the scientific method to test its basic laws or theories cannot be called “science.” For instance, theology (the study of religion) is not science because theological ideas (such as the presence of God) cannot be tested by independent observers using a replicable, precise, falsifiable, and parsimonious method. Similarly, arts, music, literature, humanities, and law are also not considered science, even though they are creative and worthwhile endeavors in their own right.

The scientific method, as applied to social sciences, includes a variety of research approaches, tools, and techniques, such as qualitative and quantitative data, statistical analysis, experiments, field surveys, case research, and so forth. Most of this book is devoted to learning about these different methods. However, recognize that the scientific method operates primarily at the empirical level of research, i.e., how to make observations and analyze and interpret these observations. Very little of this method is directly pertinent to the theoretical level, which is really the more challenging part of scientific research.

Types of Scientific Research

Depending on the purpose of research, scientific research projects can be grouped into three types: exploratory, descriptive, and explanatory. Exploratory research is often conducted in new areas of inquiry, where the goals of the research are: (1) to scope out the magnitude or extent of a particular phenomenon, problem, or behavior, (2) to generate some initial ideas (or “hunches”) about that phenomenon, or (3) to test the feasibility of undertaking a more extensive study regarding that phenomenon. For instance, if the citizens of a country are generally dissatisfied with governmental policies regarding during an economic recession, exploratory research may be directed at measuring the extent of citizens’ dissatisfaction, understanding how such dissatisfaction is manifested, such as the frequency of public protests, and the presumed causes of such dissatisfaction, such as ineffective government policies in dealing with inflation, interest rates, unemployment, or higher taxes. Such research may include examination of publicly reported figures, such as estimates of economic indicators, such as gross domestic product (GDP), unemployment, and consumer price index, as archived by third-party sources, obtained through interviews of experts, eminent economists, or key government officials, and/or derived from studying historical examples of dealing with similar problems. This research may not lead to a very accurate understanding of the target problem, but may be worthwhile in scoping out the nature and extent of the problem and serve as a useful precursor to more in-depth research.

Research in motion network & wireless cards driver download windows 7

Descriptive research is directed at making careful observations and detailed documentation of a phenomenon of interest. These observations must be based on the scientific method (i.e., must be replicable, precise, etc.), and therefore, are more reliable than casual observations by untrained people. Examples of descriptive research are tabulation of demographic statistics by the United States Census Bureau or employment statistics by the Bureau of Labor, who use the same or similar instruments for estimating employment by sector or population growth by ethnicity over multiple employment surveys or censuses. If any changes are made to the measuring instruments, estimates are provided with and without the changed instrumentation to allow the readers to make a fair before-and-after comparison regarding population or employment trends. Other descriptive research may include chronicling ethnographic reports of gang activities among adolescent youth in urban populations, the persistence or evolution of religious, cultural, or ethnic practices in select communities, and the role of technologies such as Twitter and instant messaging in the spread of democracy movements in Middle Eastern countries.

Explanatory research seeks explanations of observed phenomena, problems, or behaviors. While descriptive research examines the what, where, and when of a phenomenon, explanatory research seeks answers to why and how types of questions. It attempts to “connect the dots” in research, by identifying causal factors and outcomes of the target phenomenon. Examples include understanding the reasons behind adolescent crime or gang violence, with the goal of prescribing strategies to overcome such societal ailments. Most academic or doctoral research belongs to the explanation category, though some amount of exploratory and/or descriptive research may also be needed during initial phases of academic research. Seeking explanations for observed events requires strong theoretical and interpretation skills, along with intuition, insights, and personal experience. Those who can do it well are also the most prized scientists in their disciplines.

History of Scientific Thought

Before closing this chapter, it may be interesting to go back in history and see how science has evolved over time and identify the key scientific minds in this evolution. Although instances of scientific progress have been documented over many centuries, the terms “science,” “scientists,” and the “scientific method” were coined only in the 19 th century. Prior to this time, science was viewed as a part of philosophy, and coexisted with other branches of philosophy such as logic, metaphysics, ethics, and aesthetics, although the boundaries between some of these branches were blurred.

In the earliest days of human inquiry, knowledge was usually recognized in terms of theological precepts based on faith. This was challenged by Greek philosophers such as Plato, Aristotle, and Socrates during the 3 rd century BC, who suggested that the fundamental nature of being and the world can be understood more accurately through a process of systematic logical reasoning called rationalism . In particular, Aristotle’s classic work Metaphysics (literally meaning “beyond physical [existence]”) separated theology (the study of Gods) from ontology (the study of being and existence) and universal science (the study of first principles, upon which logic is based). Rationalism (not to be confused with “rationality”) views reason as the source of knowledge or justification, and suggests that the criterion of truth is not sensory but rather intellectual and deductive, often derived from a set of first principles or axioms (such as Aristotle’s “law of non-contradiction”).

Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download Windows 7

The next major shift in scientific thought occurred during the 16 th century, when British philosopher Francis Bacon (1561-1626) suggested that knowledge can only be derived from observations in the real world. Based on this premise, Bacon emphasized knowledge acquisition as an empirical activity (rather than as a reasoning activity), and developed empiricism as an influential branch of philosophy. Bacon’s works led to the popularization of inductive methods of scientific inquiry, the development of the “scientific method” (originally called the “Baconian method”), consisting of systematic observation, measurement, and experimentation, and may have even sowed the seeds of atheism or the rejection of theological precepts as “unobservable.”

Empiricism continued to clash with rationalism throughout the Middle Ages, as philosophers sought the most effective way of gaining valid knowledge. French philosopher Rene Descartes sided with the rationalists, while British philosophers John Locke and David Hume sided with the empiricists. Other scientists, such as Galileo Galilei and Sir Issac Newton, attempted to fuse the two ideas into natural philosophy (the philosophy of nature), to focus specifically on understanding nature and the physical universe, which is considered to be the precursor of the natural sciences. Galileo (1564-1642) was perhaps the first to state that the laws of nature are mathematical, and contributed to the field of astronomy through an innovative combination of experimentation and mathematics.

In the 18 th century, German philosopher Immanuel Kant sought to resolve the dispute between empiricism and rationalism in his book Critique of Pure Reason , by arguing that experience is purely subjective and processing them using pure reason without first delving into the subjective nature of experiences will lead to theoretical illusions. Kant’s ideas led to the development of German idealism , which inspired later development of interpretive techniques such as phenomenology, hermeneutics, and critical social theory.

At about the same time, French philosopher Auguste Comte (1798–1857), founder of the discipline of sociology, attempted to blend rationalism and empiricism in a new doctrine called positivism . He suggested that theory and observations have circular dependence on each other. While theories may be created via reasoning, they are only authentic if they can be verified through observations. The emphasis on verification started the separation of modern science from philosophy and metaphysics and further development of the “scientific method” as the primary means of validating scientific claims. Comte’s ideas were expanded by Emile Durkheim in his development of sociological positivism (positivism as a foundation for social research) and Ludwig Wittgenstein in logical positivism.

Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download

In the early 20 th century, strong accounts of positivism were rejected by interpretive sociologists (antipositivists) belonging to the German idealism school of thought. Positivism was typically equated with quantitative research methods such as experiments and surveys and without any explicit philosophical commitments, while antipositivism employed qualitative methods such as unstructured interviews and participant observation. Even practitioners of positivism, such as American sociologist Paul Lazarsfield who pioneered large-scale survey research and statistical techniques for analyzing survey data, acknowledged potential problems of observer bias and structural limitations in positivist inquiry. In response, antipositivists emphasized that social actions must be studied though interpretive means based upon an understanding the meaning and purpose that individuals attach to their personal actions, which inspired Georg Simmel’s work on symbolic interactionism, Max Weber’s work on ideal types, and Edmund Husserl’s work on phenomenology.

Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download Windows 10

In the mid-to-late 20 th century, both positivist and antipositivist schools of thought were subjected to criticisms and modifications. British philosopher Sir Karl Popper suggested that human knowledge is based not on unchallengeable, rock solid foundations, but rather on a set of tentative conjectures that can never be proven conclusively, but only disproven. Empirical evidence is the basis for disproving these conjectures or “theories.” This metatheoretical stance, called postpositivism (or postempiricism), amends positivism by suggesting that it is impossible to verify the truth although it is possible to reject false beliefs, though it retains the positivist notion of an objective truth and its emphasis on the scientific method.

Research In Motion Network & Wireless Cards Driver Download 64-bit

Likewise, antipositivists have also been criticized for trying only to understand society but not critiquing and changing society for the better. The roots of this thought lie in Das Capital , written by German philosophers Karl Marx and Friedrich Engels, which critiqued capitalistic societies as being social inequitable and inefficient, and recommended resolving this inequity through class conflict and proletarian revolutions. Marxism inspired social revolutions in countries such as Germany, Italy, Russia, and China, but generally failed to accomplish the social equality that it aspired. Critical research (also called critical theory) propounded by Max Horkheimer and Jurgen Habermas in the 20 th century, retains similar ideas of critiquing and resolving social inequality, and adds that people can and should consciously act to change their social and economic circumstances, although their ability to do so is constrained by various forms of social, cultural and political domination. Critical research attempts to uncover and critique the restrictive and alienating conditions of the status quo by analyzing the oppositions, conflicts and contradictions in contemporary society, and seeks to eliminate the causes of alienation and domination (i.e., emancipate the oppressed class). More on these different research philosophies and approaches will be covered in future chapters of this book.





Coments are closed